This is all too common in scientific communication... >> Coulour m...
Most readers will only read the title, abstract and figures of a sc...
More on color vision... >>"Isaac Newton discovered that white l...
Has anyone got good examples of where these changed?
PERSPECTIVE
The misuse of colour in science communication
Fabio Crameri
1
, Grace E. Shephard
1
& Philip J. Heron
2
The accurate representation of data is essential in science communication. However, colour
maps that visually distort data through uneven colour gradients or are unreadable to those
with colour-vision deciency remain prevalent in science. These include, but are not limited
to, rainbow-like and redgreen colour maps. Here, we present a simple guide for the scientic
use of colour. We show how scientically derived colour maps report true data variations,
reduce complexity, and are accessible for people with colour-vision deciencies. We highlight
ways for the scientic community to identify and prevent the misuse of colour in science, and
call for a proactive step away from colour misuse among the community, publishers, and
the press.
V
ision is one of the most fundamental means of communication. It is (or should be) in
every scientists best intention to make gures and their content as accurate and easily
understandable as possible. One of the most powerful aspects of images is colour, which
in turn transforms information into meaning. The visual evaluation of a colour gradient is
important to a variety of different elds such as the rst direct impression of a black hole
1
, the
mapping of votes cast in political elections
2,3
, the planning of an expensive rover route on
Martian topography
4
, the essential communication of climate change
5,6
, or the critical diagnosis
of heart disease
7
. However, when colours are used incorrectly, this can lead to the effective
manipulation of data (e.g., by highlighting some data over others), the oversight of the needs of
those with colour vision deciencies, and the removal of meaning when printed in black and
white (Supplementary Note 1).
As science has become more prevalent in mainstream culture, it is not only the scientic
community that suffers due to the use of poor colour choices, but also the wider public. Colour
maps, therefore, are a crucial intersection between science and society. For instance, weather
forecasts and hazard maps are two examples of immediately societal-relevant data sets that are
also repeat offenders for use of the rainbow-like colour maps. Given the (daily) importance of
these scientic topics, the underlying data should be conveyed in a universal manner. However,
the colour-vision decient fraction of the population is excluded and therefore unable to process
this critical information. Furthermore, zones of danger, such as the boundaries of a hurricane
track or current virus spread, are often based on uneven colour gradients to accentuate their
importance. Using an uneven colour gradient is not an action without consequences, including
those with signicant nancial or life-threatening consequences. Decisions based on data being
unfairly represented could produce, for instance, a Martian rover being sent over terrain that is
too steep as the topography was inaccurately visualised, or a medical worker making an
incomplete or inaccurate diagnosis based on uneven colour gradients.
Although some scientic communities have largely moved away from using distorting colour
maps, such as rainbow, there are numerous signs of bad habits returning en masse
8
. Unfortu-
nately, the previous efforts within specic disciplines to discredit rainbow-like maps appear to
https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7
OPEN
1
Centre for Earth Evolution and Dynamics (CEED), University of Oslo, Postbox 1028, Blindern, 0315 Oslo, Norway.
2
Department of Earth Sciences, Durham
University, Durham, UK.
email: fabio.crameri@geo.uio.no
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 1
1234567890():,;
have not trickled through to all scientic leaders, publishers, and
software designers. For most scientists, the choice of colour maps
has become almost passive, with the unscientic rainbow-like
colour palette being commonplace. Similarly, colour maps that
pair redgreen are also problematic, but remain widely used. At
one point, most of the common software programmes applied
rainbow as their default palette (e.g., MatLab, Paraview, VisAd,
IrisExplorer) despite issues surrounding colour maps like rain-
bow, and variations thereof, being known for some time
920
.
So, whats the problem with these colour maps? Even though
rainbow colour maps might reect aesthetic attractiveness, the
extreme values in the standard Red-Green-Blue (RGB) are very
dominant and can, therefore, distract from the underlying visual
message
21
. In rainbow colour maps, the yellow is the brightest
colour and attracts the eye the most
22,23
(see Box 1), but it is
neither at the end nor the centre of the colour map, while its
greenish shades form a wide band with low perceived colour
contrast (Supplementary Fig. 1). Hence, such an arrangement of
colours can unfairly highlight a particular section of the para-
meter space while obscuring other parts (Fig. 1). Building a colour
map on a purely physical rather than a perceptual basis sig-
nicantly alters how we perceive data; it adds articial boundaries
to some parts of the data range, hiding small-scale variations
elsewhere, it prevents any visual intuitive order occurring in the
data set, and renders the data unreadable for readers with com-
mon colour-vision deciencies (Fig. 2).
Colour maps that include both red and green colours with
similar lightness cannot be read by a large fraction of the read-
ership (Fig. 2). The general estimate is that worldwide 0.5% of
women and 8% of men are subject to a colour-vision deciency
(CVD; e.g., refs.
24,25
, and references therein). While these
numbers are lower and almost disappear in populations from
sub-Saharan Africa, they are likely signicantly higher in popu-
lations with a larger fraction of white (Caucasian) people as, for
example, in Scandinavia
26
. It is needless to state that scientic
results should be accessible to as many people as possible. CVD-
friendliness is, therefore, an aspect that should be important to all
researchers, as well as publishers
18,27,28
.
For the casual reader, it might appear curious that the com-
munity of scientists, a group of people who are usually more
critically inclined, fail to condemn the proliferation among
themselves of rainbow and other, similarly unt, colour maps.
Based on this prevalence of unscientic colour maps, a basic
understanding of their nature (Supplementary Fig. 2) and their
dynamics seems to be missing in the academic community.
Colour map choice is often made passively and is not subject to
the same scrutiny as other data methodologies. Rainbows eye-
grabbing nature, ease of use, and the inertia of both software
products and scientic group leaders, allows the scientic com-
munity to remain attached to this unscientic colour map
29
.Asa
result, pointing out colour map aws to the science community
through scientic peer-review remains a challenging task, as
BOX 1:
|
Human colour perception
Colour is not inherent in objects. A perceived colour is the portion of light that is reected from a surface and translated into a specic colour by our
eyes and brain (Box Fig. 1). A certain colour is perceived via its hue, the true tint (i.e., yellow, orange, red, violet, blue or green), and its luminosity (the
measure of how bright or dark a hue is). Within the eye, one type of light receptors, the rod cells, process achromatic information (i.e., lightness or grey-
scale vision), which is derived mostly from the lights energy. The other type, the cone cells, handle chromatic information (the hue) mostly from the
lights wavelength. The latter transmit information to the brain and create the sensation of colour that is so familiar to most of us.
Two thirds of all cone cells process longer wavelengths of light (i.e., colours like red, orange, yellow), which allows the human eye to perceive more
colour detail across warmer colours than for cooler colours
54
. Greenish colour gradients tend to under-represent a given data variation compared to
yellow-red gradients. Three types of cone cells (for short, medium and, long wavelengths) build the trichromatic visual system that can, as a whole,
represent all colours in our visual spectrum
5558
.
The physiological prerequisites for perceiving colour suggest that there is no uniform colour perception among individuals
59
; several physiological
deviations can lead to a shift in colour perception, which, in general, is hardly measurable
60
. However, it is not unlikely that at least one of the cone cell
types is altered, defect, or even absent, which induces a signicant shift in colour vision. Such a shift is commonly referred to as colour-vision deciency
(CVD), or colour blindness, and can be modelled by a given combination of the three fundamental spectral sensitivity functions representing short,
medium and long wavelengths of the light
55
(as represented in Fig. 2). The most common form of colour-vision deciency is the redgreen
dichromatism, called deuteranomaly, concerning the M-cone and causing red and green to appear indistinguishable. Protanomaly, concerning the L-
cone, and tritanomaly, concerning the S-cone cause reduced sensitivity to red and blue light, respectively. Total colour blindness is, fortunately, very
rare, but does exist.
Box Fig. 1: Simplied schematic of human colour perception. The perception of a certain colour, or colour gradient, by the optical cortex depends not
only on the optical properties of the object, but also on the optical properties of both light source and object background, and the eyes light receptors
(rod and cone cells for short, medium and long wavelengths).
“Coloured”
object
Object
Background
Light source Optical cortex
Optical nerveEye lens
Cone cells
(S, M, L)
PERSPECTIVE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7
2 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications
jet
Original
batlow
a
b
c
Fig. 1 The superiority of scientically derived colour maps. By knowing what something looks like in advance, the distortion by unscientic colour maps,
like jet or rainbow, becomes instantly obvious. The look of scientic data is, however, usually unknown a priori, which makes the distortion of an
unscientic colour map, and the true data representation of a scientically derived colour map, like batlow
41
, less apparent. Marie Skłodowska-Curie, as
originally photographed by Henri Manuel around 1920, the Earth from space, and an apple are shown a in their original images and b in distorted and c in
undistorted colour versions. Inferring the true picture from an unscientically (e.g., jet) coloured data set is incomparably harder than from a data set
represented in a perceptually uniform and ordered colour map, like batlow
41
.
viridis
Colour-vision deficient (CVD)
Colour-blind
Deuteranopia
Protanopia Tritanopia
CVD-friendly
Grey scale
cividis
kry
thermal
batlow
jet
Fig. 2 Colour vision tests. Available perceptually uniform colour maps versus the non-uniform rainbow (i.e., jet; bottom row) as seen with either of the
three common forms of human colour-vision deciency (deuteranopia, protanopia, and tritanopia), and for grey-scale (representing total colour-blindness
or simple black-and-white prints). Rainbow, the most-widely used colour map, fails to reproduce a meaningful smooth gradient, yet the other colour
maps (see Box 2) are all universally readable.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 PERSPECTIVE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 3
requests or suggestions to change the colour palette are often met
with rebuttal, misunderstanding, or are simply ignored.
Here, we highlight the importance of the scientic use of colour
in data visualisation. We summarise methods to generate colour
maps that deliver equal colour gradients all along the colour axis,
to prevent data distortion. Various types of scientically derived
colour maps are readily and freely available (Box 2), and are the
only way to intuitively and inclusively represent data without any
blind interpretation. We outline key guidelines for choosing sci-
entic colour maps that most accurately represent your data, and
highlight how to spot common unscientic colour maps so they
can be avoided. Finally, we call for a universal switch by the
science community to adopt scientically derived colour maps.
Individuals, publishers and press should take a proactive
approach to spot unscientic use of colour and so prevent the
dissemination of visually distorted data.
The importance of colour to prevent data distortion. Scientic
data should be displayed without the addition of visually articial
features or subtraction of real details, while also being universally
readable and intuitive. A perceptually uniform colour map
weights the same data variation equally all across the dataspace,
while other colour maps (such as rainbow) interpret some small
data variation to be more important than others (Fig. 3). For such
unscientic, perceptually non-uniform colour maps (like rain-
bow), the interpretation is performed blindly (i.e., by the colour
map instead of by the author) as the author or reader is not aware
of the exact visual distortion that is introduced by the colour map
(Fig. 3). Any scientic study drawing conclusions from a given set
of data is, therefore, strongly dependent on the perceptual uni-
formity of the colour palette applied and, with it, author and
reader become captive to a blind interpretation (for various
examples, see Supplementary Note 1). Such a colour-introduced
blind interpretation can diverge from an objective representation
by more than seven percent of the displayed data variation
30
(Fig. 3ef). A at slope on Mars can become visually distorted by
the most prominent local gradients in colour rather than data,
and then appears like rough terrain, while rough terrain con-
versely might be interpreted as a at slope (compare Supple-
mentary Fig. 7a, b), which is suboptimal with regards to a
Martian exploration.
Scientically derived colour maps are perceptually uniform.
This means the same data variation is weighted equally across the
dataspace, and so the true data variation is accurately represented
without unnecessary visual error (Fig. 3a, c, e, g, i). Just like a
spatial x-, y-, or z-axis needs to have equal spacing between all
axis tick marks (Fig. 3g, h), a colour axis (commonly termed
colour bar; Supplementary Fig. 1) needs to have equidistant
colour gradients. In other words, a certain data variation (e.g., a
5 °C temperature drop) should appear the same no matter
whether it occurs at low temperatures (10 °C) or high
temperatures (30 °C).
Design and implementation of scientically derived colour
maps. While understanding the importance of perceptually uni-
form colour maps is simple, creating them can be complicated.
Quantifying perceptual colour gradients is challenging because
the complex nature of the human eye and brain has to be con-
sidered (Box 1). However, extensive research has been conducted
to understand and provide guidelines for an optimal colour map
design (see description in Methods and refs.
1820,29,3135
), and
today, various freely available tools exist to create perceptually
uniform colour palettes. In Box 2, we have compiled freely
available scientic colour map toolkits, and described the benets
BOX 2
|
Freely available scientically derived colour maps and toolkits
Scientically derived colour maps should be easily recognisable, and also be freely and readily available. While there are many online sources to
download pre-made colour maps, and toolboxes to create colour maps, only few offer all the aspects required for use in science communication. Below,
we mention the most accessible and/or robustly documented sources of scientically derived colour maps, and outline their individual properties.
Colorbrewer: The Colorbrewer colour maps (available at ref.
61
) developed by Cynthia Brewer are provided through an online tool to manually
produce and export a variety of discrete colour maps, which can, optionally, be colour-vision deciency friendly and exported to a given a variety of
formats. With the main aim being cartography, the online tool offers sequential, diverging and categorical (or in other words, qualitative) colour
maps, but does not currently offer them in a continuous type.
MPL (Matplotlib): The MPL colour maps (available at ref.
62
) developed by Stéfan van der Walt and Nathaniel Smith. MPL maps aim for the most
accurate perceptual uniformity with its widely applied colour maps being: viridis, magma, plasma and inferno. These maps have spearheaded the way
towards more scientic colour mapping. The MPL colour maps are all sequential and continuous only. The MPL colour maps are openly available
(currently for use with Python) and often built into software.
Cividis: The cividis colour map
38
(available at ref.
63
), developed by Jamie R. Nuñez and colleagues, aims to represent an almost identical
appearance for redgreen colour-vision deciencies, the closest of all currently available colour maps, while also being perceptually uniform. The
colour-vision deciency friendly, sequential and continuous colour map is currently available as a standard colour array.
CMOcean (colormaps inspired by oceanography): The CMOcean colour maps
20
(available at ref.
64
), developed by Kristen M. Thyng and
colleagues, aim to provide the most intuitive colours for a given suite of physical parameters, while now also being perceptually uniform. A variety of
continuous sequential, diverging and cyclic colour maps are provided to allow for an intuitive, true representation of a given physical parameter eld.
The CMOcean colour maps are available in a large variety of le formats.
CET (Centre for Exploration Targeting): The CET colour maps
32
(available at ref.
65
), developed by Peter Kovesi, aim to offer a large choice of the
most common colour combinations in a wide variety of data formats. Many of the offered colour maps feature perceptual uniformity, although not all
of them to the highest standards. The CET colour maps are continuous and cover sequential, diverging, and cyclic classes.
Scientic colour maps: The Scientic colour maps
30
(available at ref.
41
, and permanently archived at ref.
52
) are perceptually uniform (based on the
underlying methodology of the CET colour maps
65
), perceptually ordered, colour-vision deciency and colour-blind friendly, readable in black and
white prints, and, if applied properly, also data set specic and parameter intuitive. The Scientic colour maps include sequential, diverging, and
cyclic palettes, which are also provided as discrete and categorical palettes, and are available in a multitude of different le formats. They are also
available through external routines and as built-in versions in a variety of software programmes. In contrast to others, the Scientic colour map
package includes individual colour map diagnostics, and is versioned on a long-term online repository so individual versions can be accurately cited,
which allows active developments from the community (e.g., improve their perceptual uniformity to the latest standards), and aids overall scientic
reproducibility.
PERSPECTIVE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7
4 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications
and limitations of each toolkit, to aid users in designing and/or
selecting the most appropriate and accurate colour map for
their data.
Perceptual uniformity is a crucial property for colour maps
used in science, but by no means the only property to care about.
Perceptual colour order is another important aspect in scientic
colour map design, as it ensures the colour gradient is easily and
intuitively understandable and allows qualitative understanding
of a data set (Fig. 4). To achieve perceptual order, both lightness
and brightness should increase linearly to avoid the perception of
articial gradients and to easily discern and compare signicant
values. The heated black-body radiation palette, for which the
colours can be easily ordered from black-red-orange-yellow (or
vice versa), is one example. Perceptual order is, therefore, a great
asset to a colour map by emphasising gradients and pattern(s) in
data. Moreover yet, colour maps specically created for certain
data sets (e.g., ref.
36
) by no means guarantee their perceptual
uniformity. Perceptual uniformity and intuitive colour order are
both needed to prevent bias percolating into colour maps. This is
also the case for so-called improved rainbow-like maps such as
Googles Turbo
37
. Although Turbo appears to meet perceptual
order, the perceptual uniformity requirement of a science-ready
colour map is not met due to its non-uniform lightness spectra.
In addition, colour maps should be universally readable, and
they can be mathematically optimised to account for colour-
vision deciency using modern colour appearance models
38
(Fig. 2 and Box 1). As well as making colour palettes readable
for readers with variable vision capabilities, it is also advantageous
to make them readable for completely colour-blind readers. In
contrast to other colour palettes, a colour map for scienti c use
should feature a uniform gradient across the whole colour axis.
With an even, monotonic lightness gradient (Supplementary
Fig. 1a), a colour palette remains readable (as well as perceptually
uniform and ordered) even after a conversion to grey scale,
assuming no complicated grey-scale lter is used.
To ach ieve the best possible data representation, colour
palettes need to effectively convey the underlying data and its
nature. This can be achieved by choosing the most appropriate
colourmapclassandtype(Fig.5 and outlined in detail
in Supplementary Notes 2 and 3). If the data is divergent about
a central value (e.g., centred about zero), a divergent or a multi-
sequential colour map should be c hosen that clearly and
intuitively distinguishes either side of the axis (Fig. 5,andFig.6
for a detailed user guide).
An intuitive colour gradient becomes imperative if data are
displayed without the provision of the colour bar (the axis relating
colours to data values). Unfortunately, missing colour bars are
more common than most would expect, as scales are sometimes
cropped out or omitted during reproduction or subsequent
dissemination (e.g., refs.
1,4,39
). It is imperative to ensure the
batlow jet
2
1.5
1
0.5
Δ
Δ
E
CIEDE2000
0
10.0
200
150
100
50
0
50
Δ
E
Cumulative
0
10
5
0123
batlow represented as a position axis
batlow representing a linear graph jet representing a linear graph
j
i
456789
10
0123
jet represented as a position axis
45 678 9
10
Visual error %
Colour bar index Colour bar index
0.2
0
5
8.15
10
2
1.5
1
0.5
0
g
h
f
e
c
d
b
a
Fig. 3 Colour map measure, distortion, and error. a, b The incremental lightness difference, ΔE, here using the CIEDE2000 formulation (see Methods), is
a measure for the perceptual colour difference along the colour map. For a perceptually uniform colour map, ΔE
CIEDE2000
should be equal all along the
colour map (i.e., a at graph; a). Using c, d the cumulative colour lightness difference, ΔE
Cumulative
, it is possible to extract e, f the resulting visual error in
percentage of total data variation. For scientically derived colour maps like batlow
41
, the resulting error introduced to the data by the colouring is
negligibly small as g the incremental data variation is represented equally all along the axis, and a linear data gradient, therefore, appears linear. Put
differently, i a at line looks at. For non-scientic colour maps, like jet, h data gradients are unevenly represented and f visual error can be >7% of the
displayed data variation such that j a linear graph (e.g., a at line), for example, becomes unrecognisable.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 PERSPECTIVE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 5
colour bar is included on all gures where a colour scale is used, as
even the most intuitive colour map could be rendered useless with
no colour bar for the reader to refer to. Excluding a colour bar
would be equivalent to not including the axis labels and tick marks
on the x-ory-axis of a plot. Additionally, attention has to be paid
to the lightness (i.e., light or dark) of the background on which the
colours are displayed. Background creates contrast, in lightness
and colour, to the displayed data (Supplementary Fig. 3 and
Supplementary Movie 1).
To optimise the use of the currently available perceptually
uniform colour maps listed in Box 2, t here are certain
requirements to prevent them from becoming distorted (e.g.,
ref.
30
). As the colour bar has to be handled similarly to the
position axis, parts of a scientically derived colour map cannot
AA
A
A
B
B
Colour & lightness
Colour & lightness
Lightness
Colour
Colour & lightness
Incremental contrast
Intuitive order
Variable
Unique
Unclear
Uniform
B
B
C
C
C
A
A
A
B
B
C
C
D
D
E
E
F
F
G
G
H
H
I
A
B
C
D
E
F
G
H
I
I
A
B
CDEF
HH
I
ABCDEFGHI
A BB CC
C
A
A
B
B
C
C
a
c
db
Fig. 4 Perceptual uniformity and order. A constant incremental colour and lightness contrast along a colour map is a proxy for its perceptual uniformity.
a While a certain incremental data variation is either under- or strongly overrepresented with jet (a.k.a. rainbow) depending on the colour map segment, it
is instead b evenly represented all along a colour bar when using a scientic colour map like batlow
41
, due to its uniform colour and lightness contrast.
Perceptual order is given when individual colours of a colour map can be sequentially ordered effortlessly without consulting the colour bar. While c a
sequential ordering is not intuitively possible for jet (a.k.a. rainbow), it is d possible to sequentially order individual colours of a scientically derived colour
map like batlow
41
, thanks to its constant lightness gradient.
Class
batlow
roma
romao romao10
oleron oleron10
roma10
batlow10
batlows
Continuous
Sequential
Diverging
Multi-sequential
Cyclic
Discrete Categorical
Type
Fig. 5 Colour map classes and types. The various classes of colour maps (sequential; diverging; multi-sequential; cyclic) and types (continuous; discrete;
categorical). Only sequential colour maps can be faithfully applied to categorical types of data.
PERSPECTIVE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7
6 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications
be subsequently deformed by being partly squeezed or
elongated. If the spacing between a position axis ticks were
squeezed or elongated, it would visually distort the data in the
gure. Simply put, a linear grap h would no longer appear linear
(Fig. 3h, j). Therefore, altering any available scientically
derived colour map is not recommended. Additionally,
avoiding certain types of graphs and plots is important to not
alter the local perception of individual colours. Common
heatmaps with dire ctly connected colour tiles, as opposed to
tiles with gaps in between, can alter individual colours
signicantly
40
(Supplementary Fig. 3).
How to recognise an unscientic colour map.Itisthe
responsibility of individuals , publisher s, and the press to
prevent the dissemination of visually distorted data and to take
a proactive approach in spotting the unscienticuseofcolour.
There are straightforward checks to recognise the unscientic
use of colour that are handy for users, readers, reviewers and
editors:
1. The colour bar should be perceptually uniform to prevent
data distortion and visual error. This means the perceptual
colour differences between all neighbouring colours should
appear the same. If two neighbouring colours have a
different variation compared to other neighbouring colours
(e.g., two greenish versus two yellowish-reddish colours in
rainbow colour maps), the colour bar is perceptually non-
uniform and not scientic (as shown in Fig. 4).
Given dataset
Ordered
Centric value
TYPE
CLASS
Diverging/
converging
A
Important small
variations?
Empty values
The data parameter
has intuitive colours
Centric value
Light figure
background
Light figure
background
Diverging
Continuous
Use light
coloured
centre
colour map
Use dark
coloured
centre
colour map
Use light
part for
high values
Use dark
part for
high values
Use colour
map with
parameter-
intuitive
colours
Discrete
Prevent colour map
containing the figure
background colour
Categorical
Multi-
sequential
Cyclic
Sequential
Circular/
periodic
B
No
Has the data an order?
(e.g., temperature range)
Has the data a central value?
(e.g., zero in vertical velocity)
A
Is the data ordered
relative to a central value?
(e.g., residual temperature)
B
Is the data periodic
(e.g., orientation)
Should small data
variations be visible?
Does the data contain
empty values like NaNs?
(e.g., measured surface
temperature)
Is there an intuitive
colouring for the
parameter? (e.g., blue to
red for temperature)
Has the data a central
value? (e.g., zero in
vertical velocity)
Is the plot
background
light?
No
No
No
No
No
No
No
No
No
Ye s
Ye s
Ye s
Ye s
Ye s
CHOOSING COLOUR MAP CLASS AND TYPE
CHOOSING COLOUR COMBINATION
Ye s
Ye s
Ye s
Ye s
Ye s
Fig. 6 Guideline for choosing the right scientic colour map. For effective data representation, the nature of a given data set has to be matched by a
suitable colour map class, type, and colour combination.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 PERSPECTIVE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 7
2. The colour map should not contain red and green at a
similar luminosity (Box 1 for denitions). If this is the case,
it can be assumed that these two colours cannot be
distinguished by a large fraction of the readership and,
therefore, fails as a scientic way of displaying data.
3. The common rainbow colour map should not be used in
data visualisation. There is not a single rainbow colour map
with similarly bright colours across the colour bar that
comes close to being scientic (e.g., ref.
32
).
4. The most secure test is to discard any colour map that is not
described as scientically derived (for example, is not listed
in Box 2), as it should be both perceptually uniform and
CVD friendly.
A proactive step forward for the science community.Itis
important to maintain the progress that has already been made
across the scientic community (in particular in the elds of
Earth, Space, and Climate science). Recently, a number of sig-
nicant scientic achievements have been based upon non-
distorting and universally readable scientically derived colour
maps, such as the rst ever observation-based visualisation of a
black hole
1
, for which a lajolla-like colour map
41
was used, and
the effective visualisation of climate change on a local, regional,
and global level
39
, which applied a vik-like colour map
41
. How-
ever, there are danger signs that this progress could be lost.
Unscientic colour maps are still often set as the default in
software packages, which in turn also renders them a prevalent
choice among many academic group leaders. Furthermore,
unscientic colour maps are still accepted and published by
academic journals. This combination leads users, students, and
readers into believing that these choices are based on informed
decisions, and that there are no fundamental issues with
unscientic colour maps. Given this current landscape, it is an
instructive reminder that new generations of scientists have to be
made aware of the importance of scientically derived colour
maps and the pitfalls of those that are not.
Transferring the knowledge and awareness about the impor-
tance of scientically derived colour maps to new generations of
scientists is, therefore, a key goal. Teaching is a frontline tool in
building solid visualisation skills. Indeed, learning and applying
scientic data visualisation should be a requirement to receive
BSc, MSc, and PhD degrees. Here, we provide an instructive user
guide for choosing and applying a suitable scientic colour map
(Fig. 6), which should be part of every research ofce, and
possibly even desk space. We provide a poster (Supplementary
Data 1) that can be placed in communal areas, like near a printing
station, coffee machine or restroom, that highlights the key
advantages of scientically derived colour maps and serves as a
conversation starter.
Recommendations to colleagues via peer-review is another
critical tool to ensure the quality of our ongoing scientic visual
communication. While editors could certainly provide guiding
recommendations, it is also critical that scientically derived
colour maps are being applied already during the early diagnosis
of the data, and not just applied for publication only. Set
instructions by scientic journals or conference organisers could
remind researchers to full the graphical standards that science
needs to be built upon. It might even be useful to limit researchers
to using only a handful of hand-picked, suitable colour maps for
specic types of data and data parameters to streamline and
enhance data visualisation. This type of approach is being
developed for the next assessment reports of the Intergovern-
mental Panel on Climate Change (IPCC), in collaboration with
Melissa Gomis (Graphics and Science Communication ofcer,
IPCC WGI Technical Support Unit, Université Paris-Saclay,
France).
Presently, scientically derived colour maps are easily acces-
sible and applicable across various tools and platforms and
suitable palettes exist for any given data (Box 2). The open
availability of such colour maps ultimately leaves little legitimate
room to continue using colour maps that cause visual distortion,
are unreadable in some circumstances, or exclude readers from
understanding them.
The wider scientic community needs to accept that colour
maps are a pivotal tool in scientic discovery, data visualisation
and science communication. Non-scientic colour maps require
scrutiny and rejection from the community to preserve academic
integrity. The evidence is clear, there are no more reasons to
continue using unscientic colour maps.
Methods
Dening colour spaces. Various methods and tools based on different metrics and
colour spaces (Supplementary Note 4) exist to diagnose the uniformity of a colour
palette (e.g., refs.
32,34,38
). A widely used example is the International Commission
on Illuminations uniform colour space named CIELAB UCS
4244
. CIELAB UCS
(also abbreviated from Uniform/Unied Colour Scale, Chromaticity Scale, Chro-
maticity Space) describes the complete range of colours in a perceptually uniform
rectangular coordinate system
45
and allows the creation of perceptually uniform
colour palettes
32,35,46,47
.
The colour appearance model CIECAM02
48,49
, and the perceptually uniform
colour space CIECAM02-UCS
50
based on it, is the current gold standard to
describe how we perceive colour and colour differences. CIECAM02-UCS describes
a certain colour by its lightness (L) and its redgreen (a) and yellow-blue (b)
correlates (Supplementary Figs. 4 and 5), and it represents a certain perceptual
colour variation on equal Euclidean distances across the whole space. Colour
spaces like CIECAM02-UCS make it possible to design perceptually uniform
colour maps and, also, to take colour-decient visio n into account.
Here, the CIELAB UCS colour difference metric ΔECIEDE 2000
51
is used
(Fig. 3a, b) and allows the calculation of a local lightness gradient between two
colours by
ΔE
CIEDE2000
¼½ðL
1
L
2
Þ
2
þða
1
a
2
Þ
2
þðb
1
b
2
Þ
2
1=2
;
ð1Þ
where L is the lightness of one specic colour, and a its redgreen and b its yellow-
blue correlative (Supplementary Figs. 4 and 5). This lightness difference metric can
be used to diagnose any colour map (e.g., ref.
20
) and calculate the effective,
perceptual error that is added to the underlying data by uneven colour gradients
along a colour map
30
. These errors and the resulting visual data distortion can be
signicant and make, for example, linear data gradients look like a wobbly graph
(Fig. 3).
Data availability
The scientic colour maps are openly available from ref.
41
and archived on Zenodo
52
(https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1243862). All other additional information related to the
article is provided in the Supplementary Information and Box 2.
Received: 17 January 2020; Accepted: 1 October 2020;
References
1. Akiyama, K. et al. First M87 event horizon telescope results. IV. Imaging the
central supermassive black hole. Astrophys. J. Lett. 875, L4 (2019).
2. Long, E. Election Data Visualisation (University of Plymouth, 2013).
3. Cox, A., Bostock, M., Watkins, D. & Shan, C. The Most Detailed Maps Youll
See from the Midterm Elections., https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2014/
11/04/upshot/senate-maps.html (2014).
4. Lemoine, F. G. et al. An improved solution of the gravity eld of Mars (GMM-
2B) from Mars Global Surveyor. J. Geophys. Res.: Planets 106, 2335923376
(2001).
5. Hawkins, E. Scrap rainbow colour scales. Nature 519, 291 EP (2015).
6. IPCC. Global Warming of 1.5° C: An IPCC Special Report on the Impacts of
Global Warming of 1.5° C Above Pre-industrial Levels and Related Global
Greenhouse Gas Emission Pathways, in the Context of Strengthening the Global
Response to the Threat of Climate Change, Sustainable Development, and
Efforts to Eradicate Poverty. (Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change,
2018).
PERSPECTIVE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7
8 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications
7. Borkin, M. et al. Evaluation of artery visualizations for heart disease diagnosis.
IEEE Trans. Vis. Computer Graph. 17, 24792488 (2011).
8. McNeall, D. How many Rainbows at EGU 2018,<https://bettergures.org/
2018/04/16/how-many-rainbows-at-egu-2018> (2020).
9. Bertin, J. Graphics and Graphic Information Processing. (Walter de Gruyter,
2011).
10. Bertin, J. Semiology of graphics; diagrams networks maps. (University of
Wisconsin, Press, Madison, 1983).
11. Tufte, E. R. Envisioning Information. Vol. 126 (Graphics Press, 1990).
12. Travis, D. Effective Color Displays: Theory and Practice. Vol. 1991 (Academic
press London, 1991).
13. Brewer, C. A. Color use guidelines for mapping. Vis. Mod. Cartogr. 1994,
123148 (1994).
14. MacEachren, A. M. How Maps Work: Representation, Visualization, and
Design. (Guilford Press, 2004).
15. Dent, B. D., Torguson, J. S. & Hodler, T. W. Cartography: Thematic Map
Design. Vol. 5 (WCB/McGraw-Hil l New York, 1999).
16. Robinson, A. Elements of Cartography. (John Wiley and Sons, New York,
1960).
17. Monmonier, M. How to Lie with Maps (University of Chicag o Press, 2018).
18. Light, A. & Bartlein, P. J. The end of the rainbow? Color schemes for
improved data graphics. Eos Trans. AGU 85, 385391 (2004).
19. Stauffer, R., Mayr, G. J., Dabernig, M. & Zeileis, A. Somewhere over the
rainbow: how to make effective use of colors in meteorological visualizations.
Bull. Am. Meteorological Soc. 96, 203216 (2015).
20. Thyng, K. M., Greene, C. A., Hetland, R. D., Zimmerle, H. M. & DiMarco, S.
F. True colors of oceanography: guidelines for effective and accurate colormap
selection. Oceanography 29,913 (2016). Introducing the scientically
derived cmocean colour maps for better data visualisation in oceanography
that also underline the importance of intuitive colour palettes given a certain
data parameter.
21. Cairo, A. The Functional Art: An Introduction to Information Graphics and
Visualization. (New Riders, 2012).
22. Wolfe, J. M. & Horowitz, T. S. Five factors that guide attention in visual
search. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1, 0058 (2017).
23. Alexander, K. R. & Shansky, M. S. Inuence of hue, value, and chroma on the
perceived heaviness of colors. Percept. Psychophys. 19,7274 (1976).
24. Sharpe, L. T., Stockman, A., Jägle, H. & Nathans, J. Opsin Genes, Cone
Photopigments, Color Vision, and Color Blindness. Vol. 351 (Cambridge
University Press, 1999).
25. Neitz, J. & Neitz, M. The genetics of normal and defective color vision. Vis.
Res. 51, 633651 (2011).
26. Drummond-Borg, M., Deeb, S. S. & Motulsky, A. G. Molecular patterns of X
chromosome-linked color vision genes among 134 men of European ancestry.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. 86, 983987 (1989).
27. Albrecht, M. Color blindness. Nat. Methods 7, 775 EP (2010).
28. Wong, B. Reply to \More on color blindness\. Nat. Methods 8, 891 EP
(2011).
29. Moreland, K. Why we use bad color maps and what you can do about it.
Electron. Imaging 2016,16 (2016). The widespread use of the rainbow is the
main reason scientists propagate it further, which must therefore be
considered if a scientic alternative is to be effective.
30. Crameri, F. Geodynamic diagnostics, scientic visualisation and StagLab 3.0.
Geoscientic Model Dev. 11, 25412562 (2018).
31. Silva, S., Sousa Santos, B. & Madeira, J. Using color in visualization: a survey.
Computer Graph. 35, 320333 (2011).
32. Kovesi, P. Good colour maps: how to design them. CoRR abs/1509.03700
(2015). Provides a set of colour map diagnostics and shows that the most
important factor designing a colour map is perceptually uniform lightness,
allowing to construct more consistent and reliable ternary base colours to
replace the problematic RGB primaries red, green, and blue.
33. Szar, D. A. Modeling color difference for visualization design. IEEE Trans.
Vis. Computer Graph. 24, 392401 (2018).
34. Bujack, R. et al. The Good, the bad, and the ugly: a theoretical framework for
the assessment of continuous colormaps. IEEE Trans. Vis. Computer Graph.
24, 923933 (2018).
35. Ware, C. et al. Measuring and modeling the feature detection threshold
functions of colormaps. IEEE Trans. Vis. Computer Graph. 25, 27772790
(2019).
36. Zeller, S. & Rogers, D. Visualizing science: How color determines what we see.
Eos Trans. AGU Vol. 101 (American Geophysical Union, 2020). https://doi.
org/10.1029/2020EO144330
37. Mikhailov, A. in Google AI Blog Vol. 2020 (Google, https://ai.googleblog.com/
2019/08/turbo-improved-rainbow-colormap-for.html, 2019).
38. Nuñez, J. R., Anderton, C. R. & Renslow, R. S. Optimizing colormaps with
consideration for color vision deciency to enable accurate interpretation of
scienti
c data. PLoS ONE 13, e0199239 (2018). Developed the mathematically
optimal colour map for readers with different colour-vision deciencies
named Cividis, which allows most accurate perception of data by as many
viewers as possible.
39. Hawkins, E. (http://www.climate-lab-book.ac.uk/2018/2018-visualisation-
update/, 2019).
40. Wong, B. Color coding. Nat. Methods 7, 573 EP (2010).
41. Crameri, F. Scientic Colour Maps, http://www.fabiocrameri.ch/colourmaps
(2020).
42. Smith, T. & Guild, J. The CIE colorimetric standards and their use. Trans.
Optical Soc. 33, 73 (1931).
43. C. I. E. Technical Report Colorimetry. (Commission Internationale de
lEclairage Central Bureau Vienna, Austria, 2004).
44. CIE. Part 4: CIE 1976 L* a* b* Colour space, http://www.cie.co.at/
publications/colorimetry-part-4-cie-1976-lab-colour-space (2011).
45. Hunter Associates Laboratory Inc. Brief Explanation of Delta E or Delta E*,
http://support.hunterlab.com/hc/en-us/articles/%20203023559-Brief-
Explanation-of-delta-E-or-delta-E -> (2019).
46. Meier, B. J., Spalter, A. M. & Karelitz, D. B. Interactive color palette tools.
IEEE Computer Graph. Appl. 24,6472 (2004).
47. Moreland, K. in Advances in Visual Computing (eds George Bebis et al.)
92103 (Springer Berlin Heidelberg, Berlin, Heidelberg, 2009).
48. Moroney, N. et al. in Color and Imaging Conference Vol. 2002 2327 (2002).
49. International Commission on Illumination. Technical Committee TC 8-01. in
Colour Appearance Modeling for Colour Management Systems (2004).
50. Luo, M. R., Cui, G. & Li, C. Uniform colour spaces based on CIECAM02
colour appearance model. Color Res. Appl. 31, 320330 (2006).
51. Lindbloom, B. Delta E (CIE 1976). http://www.brucelindbloom.com/index.
html?Eqn_DeltaE_CIE76.html (2019).
52. Crameri, F. Scientic colour maps: perceptually uniform and colour-vision
deciency friendly. Zenodo, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.1243862 (2018).
53. Crameri, F. DiVA: effective design for any MatLab gure. Zenodo
, https://doi.
org/10.5281/zenodo.3596368 (2020).
54. Curcio, C. A. et al. Distribution and morphology of human cone
photoreceptors stained with antiblue opsin. J. Comp. Neurol. 312, 610624
(1991).
55. Meyer, G. W. & Greenberg, D. P. Color-defective vision and computer
graphics displays. IEEE Computer Graph. Appl. 8,2840 (1988).
56. Lennie, P., Pokorny, J. & Smith, V. C. Luminance. J. Optical Soc. Am. A: Opt.
Image Sci., Vis. 10, 12831293 (1993).
57. Kaiser, P. & Boynton, R. Human Color Vision. 2nd edn, (Optical Society of
America, 1996).
58. Roorda, A., Metha, A. B., Lennie, P. & Williams, D. R. Packing arrangement of
the three cone classes in primate retina. Vis. Res. 41, 12911306 (2001).
59. Lafer-Sousa, R., Hermann, K. L. & Conway, B. R. Striking individual
differences in color perception uncovered by the dress photograph. Curr.
Biol. 25, R545R546 (2015).
60. Emery, K. J. & Webster, M. A. Individual differences and their implications for
color perception. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 30,2833 (2019).
61. Brewer, C. A. Colorbrewer Colour Maps, https://colorbrewer2.org/ (2020).
62. van der Walt, S. & Smith, N. MPL Colour Maps, https://bids.github.io/
colormap (2020).
63. Nuñez, J. R., Anderton, C. R. & Renslow, R. S. Cividis colour map, PLoS ONE,
https://www.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0199239.s002 (2020).
64. Thyng, K. M., Greene, C. A., Hetland, R. D., Zimmerle, H. M. & DiMarco, S.
F. CMOcean Colour Maps, http://matplotlib.org/cmocean (2020).
65. Kovesi, P. CET Colour Maps, http://peterkovesi.com/projects/colourmaps
(2020).
Acknowledgements
We thank Melissa Gomis for valuable feedback on the user guide and Stefan Scherrer for
contributing the Supplementary Movie 1. The open-access gure design routine DiVA
53
was used. F.C. and G.E.S. acknowledge support from the Research Council of Norway
through its Centres of Excellence funding scheme, Project Number 223272. P.J.H.
receives funding from the European Unions Horizon 2020 research and innovation
programme under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie Grant Agreement 749664.
Author contributions
F.C. designed the Scientic colour maps and the gures, G.E.S. contributed to the
development and software compatibility of the Scientic colour maps, P.J.H. designed the
outreach poster. All authors conceived the study, evaluated the use of and promoted
scientically derived colour maps, and contributed to the scientic discussion and pre-
paration of the manuscript.
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 PERSPECTIVE
NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications 9
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Supplementary information is available for this paper at https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-
020-19160-7.
Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to F.C.
Reprints and permission information is available at http://www.nature.com/reprints
Publishers note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in
published maps and institutional afliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons
Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing,
adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give
appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative
Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party
material in this article are included in the articles Creative Commons license, unless
indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the
articles Creative Commons license and your intended use is not permitted by statutory
regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from
the copyright holder. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons.org/
licenses/by/4.0/.
© The Author(s) 2020
PERSPECTIVE NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7
10 NATURE COMMUNICATIONS | (2020) 11:5444 | https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-020-19160-7 | www.nature.com/naturecommunications

Discussion

This is all too common in scientific communication... >> Coulour maps that visually distort data through uneven colour gradients or are unreadable to those with colour-vision deficiency remain prevalent in science. Another major problem is "chartjunk"- coined by Edward Tufte to describe visual elements in graphs that aren't necessary to comprehend the information presented and could actually distract from the information: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Chartjunk Most readers will only read the title, abstract and figures of a scientific paper, making it crucial that the figures capture the information as accurately as possible. More on color vision... >>"Isaac Newton discovered that white light after being split into its component colors when passed through a dispersive prism could be recombined to make white light by passing them through a different prism. Photopic relative brightness sensitivity of the human visual system as a function of wavelength (luminosity function). The visible light spectrum ranges from about 380 to 740 nanometers. Spectral colors (colors that are produced by a narrow band of wavelengths) such as red, orange, yellow, green, cyan, blue, and violet can be found in this range. These spectral colors do not refer to a single wavelength, but rather to a set of wavelengths: red, 625–740 nm; orange, 590–625 nm; yellow, 565–590 nm; green, 500–565 nm; cyan, 485–500 nm; blue, 450–485 nm; violet, 380–450 nm. Wavelengths longer or shorter than this range are called infrared or ultraviolet, respectively. Humans cannot generally see these wavelengths, but other animals may." Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Color_vision Has anyone got good examples of where these changed?