Quorum decision-making facilitates information
transfer in fish shoals
Ashley J. W. Ward*
†
, David J. T. Sumpter
‡
, Iain D. Couzin
§
, Paul J. B. Hart
¶
, and Jens Krause
储
*Centre for Mathematical Biology, School of Biological Sciences, University of Sydney, Sydney, New South Wales 2006, Australia;
‡
Mathematics Department,
Uppsala University, 751 06 Uppsala, Sweden;
§
Department of Ecology and Evolutionary Ecology, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ 08544;
¶
Department of
Biology, University of Leicester, Leicester LE1 7RH, United Kingdom; and
储
Faculty of Biological Sciences, University of Leeds, Leeds LS2 9JT, United Kingdom
Edited by Simon A. Levin, Princeton University, Princeton, NJ, and approved March 21, 2008 (received for review October 31, 2007)
Despite the growing interest in collective phenomena such as ‘‘swarm
intelligence’’ and ‘‘wisdom of the crowds,’’ little is known about the
mechanisms underlying decision-making in vertebrate animal groups.
How do animals use the behavior of others to make more accurate
decisions, especially when it is not possible to identify which individ-
uals possess pertinent information? One plausible answer is that
individuals respond only when they see a threshold number of
individuals perform a particular behavior. Here, we investigate the
role of such ‘‘quorum responses’’ in the movement decisions of fish
(three-spine stickleback, Gasterosteus aculeatus). We show that a
quorum response to conspecifics can explain how sticklebacks make
collective movement decisions, both in the absence and presence of
a potential predation risk. Importantly our experimental work shows
that a quorum response can reduce the likelihood of amplification of
nonadaptive following behavior. Whereas the traveling direction of
solitary fish was strongly influenced by a single replica conspecific,
the replica was largely ignored by larger groups of four or eight
sticklebacks under risk, and the addition of a second replica was
required to exert influence on the movement decisions of such
groups. Model simulations further predict that quorum responses by
fish improve the accuracy and speed of their decision-making over
that of independent decision-makers or those using a weak linear
response. This study shows that effective and accurate information
transfer in groups may be gained only through nonlinear responses
of group members to each other, thus highlighting the importance of
quorum decision-making.
behavior 兩 collective decision-making 兩 schooling 兩 shoaling 兩 social
A
nimal groups, including humans, often exhibit complex dy-
namic patterns that emerge from local interactions among
group members (1–4). This collective behavior is of particular
interest when individuals with limited personal information use
cues and signals provided by others to decide on a course of action
(5, 6).
It has been suggested that the accuracy of decision-making
increases with group size (7). However, our understanding of
exactly how behavioral interactions scale to collective properties,
and the consequences of this process to individual survival, are
limited because of the difficulty inherent in addressing the com-
plicated feedbacks that arise from repeated social interactions (1, 4,
8): individuals both create and are influenced by their social context
(9, 10). In many social interactions, it may not be possible to identify
which individuals, if any, possess pertinent information. Simply
copying the behavior of others indiscriminately may lead to cas-
cades of information transfer where the nonadaptive behavior of
single animals or small numbers of individuals is reproduced by
many other individuals at no benefit to the copiers (11–13).
Recent advances in understanding collective decision-making
have mostly come from studies of eusocial and gregarious insects (6,
14–19). These studies have emphasized the importance of quorum
response s, where animals’ probability of exhibiting a particular
behavior is an increasing function of the number of conspecifics
already performing this behavior (4). For example, cockroaches rest
longer under shelters containing other re sting cockroaches. As a
result, they make a ‘‘consensus decision’’ to shelter together under
one of two identical shelters (15). In Temnothorax ants, the prob-
ability that an ant will carry other ants to a new nest increases with
the population of ants at that new nest (6). This quorum response
can amplify the differences in the populations at two alternative
nests, usually leading to preferential choice of the better of the two
(17, 20).
Although it is known that vertebrates do use social cues provided
by conspecifics in making decisions about foraging (21–23), coop-
eration (24, 25), the timing of activities (9) and during navigation
(26), less is known about how individual decisions contribute to, and
are influenced by, collective patterns of behavior and how these
decisions in turn facilitate individual-level adaptive response s to an
uncertain environment (4). Fish provide a promising test-bed for
new theories about collective behavior (27); aggregations of fish
may display complex collective patterns yet, because of their small
size and easy maintenance, are amenable to experimental studies.
In this study, we presented three-spine sticklebacks with a choice of
moving to one of two refugia in a simple Y-maze (see Fig. 1).
Resin-cast, replica sticklebacks, which were attached to a motor and
were towed through the maze to one of the refugia, were used in
order that we could examine their effect on the movement decisions
of the experimental fish. In the second phase of the study, we added
a potential predation threat (a resin-cast replica of a sympatric
predator) to one arm of the maze to manipulate the costs of
following. By testing experimental fish in different group sizes (one,
two, four, and eight fish) and with or without one or more replica
conspecifics, we investigated the role of quorum responses in the
movement decisions of fish.
We used a simulation model to explore whether quorum re-
sponses allow for more accurate decisions than those made by
individuals acting independently of conspecifics and how decision-
making accuracy and speed change with group size. Our model is
based on the hypothesis that the propensity to go either left or right
in the maze increases as a function of the number of individuals that
have recently gone left and right. In the absence of other individuals
having taken a particular direction, uncommitted individuals
choose the left right direction with a ratio l:r, i.e., they have a
constant probability l/(l ⫹ r) of choosing the left option and r/(l ⫹
r) of choosing the right option.
In the presence of conspecifics, an individual’s probability of
going left increases as a function of the number of individuals that
have gone left in the last T time steps and decreases with the number
that have gone either right or remain uncommitted. In addition to
T, two parameters determine the shape of this response: a, which
Author contributions: A.J.W.W. and J.K. designed research; A.J.W.W. and D.J.T.S. per-
formed research; A.J.W.W., D.J.T.S., I.D.C., P.J.B.H., and J.K. analyzed data; and A.J.W.W.,
D.J.T.S., and J.K. wrote the paper.
The authors declare no conflict of interest.
This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.
†
To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: ashleyjwward@gmail.com.
This article contains supporting information online at www.pnas.org/cgi/content/full/
0710344105/DCSupplemental.
© 2008 by The National Academy of Sciences of the USA
6948– 6953
兩
PNAS
兩
May 13, 2008
兩
vol. 105
兩
no. 19 www.pnas.org兾cgi兾doi兾10.1073兾pnas.0710344105
Downloaded from https://www.pnas.org by 213.86.169.132 on December 2, 2024 from IP address 213.86.169.132.